The Scientific Method: How Scientists Investigate the World
The scientific method is a structured process that scientists use to explore questions and explain the natural world. It allows investigations to be objective, consistent, and repeatable: three things that are essential to building reliable scientific knowledge. As new technology develops or new evidence becomes available, scientists may revise their conclusions. That’s what makes science dynamic and always improving.
The Core Steps of the Scientific Method
The scientific method is often summarized by these steps:
Observation
Question
Hypothesis
Experiment
Results
Conclusion
⚠️ Note: Although these steps are often presented in order, scientific investigations are not always linear. Scientists frequently return to earlier steps as they revise ideas and explore new questions. Science is flexible and cyclical—not just a checklist.
Observation: Noticing a Pattern
Science begins with observation—recognizing a pattern in the world around us.
Observations may be:
Direct: Something we witness ourselves using our senses (e.g., watching animal behavior or plant growth).
Indirect: Patterns noticed through outside sources like news reports, data sets, or social media.
While “direct” and “indirect” observations aren't formal scientific terms, they’re helpful to distinguish how we first notice patterns worth investigating.
Question: Wondering Why
Once a pattern is observed, scientists ask questions to understand it:
Why do giraffes have long necks?
Does drinking coffee affect your heart rate?
A strong question helps narrow the investigation and focus the hypothesis.
Hypothesis: Proposing an Explanation
A hypothesis is a proposed explanation that must be:
Testable – We must be able to gather data to examine it.
Falsifiable – There must be a possibility that evidence could contradict it.
⚠️ In science, we don’t prove a hypothesis to be absolutely true. Instead, we gather evidence to support or refute it. If evidence consistently contradicts a hypothesis, it may be rejected or revised.
Experiment: Testing the Hypothesis
Experiments help us gather evidence to test our ideas. There are two major types:
Observational Experiments – Scientists study natural conditions without changing anything.
Manipulative Experiments – Scientists intentionally change one variable to see its effect.
Key concepts:
Independent variable: What the scientist changes
Dependent variable: What the scientist measures
Control group: A baseline group with no changes applied, used for comparison
Results: Collecting and Analyzing Data
Results are the data collected during an experiment. They may be:
Quantitative: Numbers or measurements (e.g., temperature, growth)
Qualitative: Descriptions or observations (e.g., color, behavior)
Data analysis helps us see relationships such as:
Positive correlation – Both variables increase together
Negative correlation – One variable increases as the other decreases
⚠️ Be careful! Correlation does not always mean causation. Scientists must interpret data cautiously and objectively.
Conclusion: Interpreting the Evidence
After analyzing the data, scientists draw conclusions:
If the results support the hypothesis, it becomes a tentative explanation.
If they do not support the hypothesis, it can be revised, retested, or rejected.
Conclusions in science must be evidence-based and repeatable. A single experiment is never the final word; it must be replicated by others to become accepted.
This process reminds us: scientists should use evidence to form conclusions—not form conclusions and then find evidence to support them.
🦒 Example 1: Giraffes and Neck Length
Direct Observation | Observational Experiment | Hypothesis Not Supported
Observation: In the 1990s, scientists noticed that after a dry season, more giraffes with longer necks were seen. This is a direct observation (they saw it with their own eyes).
Question: Does neck length help giraffes reach more food when resources are limited?
Hypothesis: Longer necks give giraffes a competitive advantage by allowing them to feed at higher tree levels.
Prediction: If this is true, longer-necked giraffes should be observed eating from the highest tree branches.Experiment: Scientists conducted an observational study. They couldn’t manipulate giraffe necks, so they recorded feeding behavior in natural settings. They observed males and females over multiple seasons, measuring both neck length and the height of vegetation eaten.
Results: Surprisingly, most giraffes fed from lower shrubs, even when food was scarce. They typically ate at about 60% of their maximum reach.
Conclusion: The data did not support the food-competition hypothesis. This prompted scientists to explore new ideas—like mate competition—as potential evolutionary drivers.
Modern research suggests that both food competition and sexual selection may play a role. Evolution is complex, and traits often result from multiple pressures over time. This example highlights how scientific knowledge evolves as more evidence is collected.
☕ Example 2: Caffeine and Heart Rate
Indirect Observation | Manipulative Experiment | Hypothesis Supported
Observation: A student noticed that people on social media claimed they felt jittery after drinking coffee. This is an indirect observation (the student didn’t observe it firsthand but noticed a pattern through outside sources).
Question: Does drinking caffeinated coffee affect heart rate?
Hypotheses:
Drinking caffeinated coffee has an effect on heart rate.
Drinking caffeinated coffee increases heart rate.
These are two possible hypotheses the student could come up with. One focused on whether or not there would be an effect. The other focused on what the potential effect would be. Both are valid hypotheses, but the differences may result in different experiments and tests.
Experiment: The student conducted a manipulative experiment:
Group 1 drank caffeinated coffee.
Group 2 drank decaffeinated coffee (control group).
Heart rate was measured before and after for both groups.
Independent variable: Caffeine consumption (yes or no)
Dependent variable: Heart rate (beats per minute)Results: Students who drank caffeinated coffee showed an average increase in heart rate compared to those who drank decaf.
Conclusion: The results supported the hypothesis. However, scientists (and students) must remain skeptical. The experiment should be repeated to confirm results and to explore other variables—like the effect of caffeine dosage or participant age.
While this example is fictional, similar studies have been conducted. It also shows how scientific questions can come from everyday life—but claims from sources like social media should always be investigated with critical thinking and controlled experimentation.
Why the Scientific Method Matters
These two examples show how science works:
We make observations.
Ask testable questions.
Form hypotheses.
Design and carry out experiments.
Analyze results.
Draw conclusions that are open to change.
Even when a hypothesis is supported, science continues. Every experiment adds to a larger body of knowledge and leads to new questions. Scientific knowledge grows through repetition, skepticism, and revision.
With the scientific method, science remains flexible, evidence-driven, and always open to improvement.